Item request has been placed!
×
Item request cannot be made.
×
Processing Request
Limited Evidence of an Association Between Language, Literacy, and Procedural Learning in Typical and Atypical Development: A Meta-Analysis.
Item request has been placed!
×
Item request cannot be made.
×
Processing Request
- المؤلفون: Oliveira CM;Oliveira CM; Henderson LM; Henderson LM; Hayiou-Thomas ME; Hayiou-Thomas ME
- المصدر:
Cognitive science [Cogn Sci] 2023 Jul; Vol. 47 (7), pp. e13310.- نوع النشر :
Meta-Analysis; Journal Article- اللغة:
English - المصدر:
- معلومة اضافية
- المصدر: Publisher: Wiley-Blackwell Country of Publication: United States NLM ID: 7708195 Publication Model: Print Cited Medium: Internet ISSN: 1551-6709 (Electronic) Linking ISSN: 03640213 NLM ISO Abbreviation: Cogn Sci Subsets: MEDLINE
- بيانات النشر: Publication: 2009-: Hoboken, N.J. : Wiley-Blackwell
Original Publication: Norwood, N. J., Ablex Pub. Corp. - الموضوع:
- نبذة مختصرة : The ability to extract patterns from sensory input across time and space is thought to underlie the development and acquisition of language and literacy skills, particularly the subdomains marked by the learning of probabilistic knowledge. Thus, impairments in procedural learning are hypothesized to underlie neurodevelopmental disorders, such as dyslexia and developmental language disorder. In the present meta-analysis, comprising 2396 participants from 39 independent studies, the continuous relationship between language, literacy, and procedural learning on the Serial Reaction Time task (SRTT) was assessed across children and adults with typical development (TD), dyslexia, and Developmental Language Disorder (DLD). Despite a significant, but very small, relationship between procedural learning and overall language and literacy measures, this pattern was not observed at the group-level when examining TD, dyslexic, and DLD groups separately. Based on the procedural/declarative model, a positive relationship was expected between procedural learning and language and literacy measures for the typically developing group; however, no such relationship was observed. This was also the case for the disordered groups (ps > .05). Also counter to expectations, the magnitude of the relationship between procedural learning and grammar and phonology did not differ between TD and DLD (ps > .05), nor between the TD and dyslexic group on reading, spelling, and phonology (ps > .05). While lending little support to the procedural/declarative model, we consider that these results may be the consequence of poor psychometric properties of the SRTT as a measure of procedural learning.
(© 2023 The Authors. Cognitive Science published by Wiley Periodicals LLC on behalf of Cognitive Science Society (CSS).) - References: References marked with an asterisk (*) were included in the meta-analysis.
Arciuli, J. (2017). The multi-component nature of statistical learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 372(1711), 20160058. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0058.
Arnon, I. (2019). Statistical Learning, Implicit Learning, and First Language Acquisition: A Critical Evaluation of Two Developmental Predictions. Topics in Cognitive Science. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12428.
Arnon, I. (2020). Do current statistical learning tasks capture stable individual differences in children? An investigation of task reliability across modality. Behavior Research Methods, 52, (1), 68-81. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-019-01205-5.
Baird, G., Dworzynski, K., Slonims, V., & Simonoff, E. (2010). Memory impairment in children with language impairment. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 52(6), 535-540. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.03494.x.
Batterink, L. J., Paller, K. A., & Reber, P. J. (2019). Understanding the neural bases of implicit and statistical learning. Topics in Cognitive Science, 11, 482-503. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12420.
Barker, L. (2012). Defining the parameters of incidental learning on a serial reaction time (SRT) task: Do conscious rules apply? Brain Sciences, 2(4), 769-789. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci2040769.
Begg, C. B., & Mazumdar, M. (1994). Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics, 50(4), 1088. https://doi.org/10.2307/2533446.
Bogaerts, L., Siegelman, N., Ben-Porat, T., & Frost, R. (2018). Is the Hebb repetition task a reliable measure of individual differences in sequence learning? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(4), 892-905. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2017.1307432.
Bogaerts, L., Siegelman, N., & Frost, R. (2021). Statistical learning and language impairments: Toward more precise theoretical accounts. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(2), 319-337. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620953082.
Borestein, M. (2009). Effect sizes for continuous data. In H. Cooper, L. V. Hedges, & J. C. Valentine (Eds.), Handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis (pp. 221-236). Russell Sage Foundation.
Brookman, A., McDonald, S., McDonald, D., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2013). Fine motor deficits in reading disability and language impairment: Same or different? PeerJ, 1, e217. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.217.
Buchholz, J., & McKone, E. (2004). Adults with dyslexia show deficits on spatial frequency doubling and visual attention tasks. Dyslexia, 10(1), 24-43. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.263.
Christiansen, M. H., Conway, C. M., & Onnis, L. (2012). Similar neural correlates for language and sequential learning: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27(2), 231-256. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.606666.
*Clark, G. M., & Lum, J. A. G. (2017a). Procedural memory and speed of grammatical processing: Comparison between typically developing children and language impaired children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 71, 237-247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2017.10.015.
* Clark, G. M., & Lum, J. A. G. (2017b). First-order and higher order sequence learning in specific language impairment. Neuropsychology, 31(2), 149-159. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000316.
Conway, C. M., Arciuli, J., Lum, J. A. G., & Ullman, M. T. (2019). Seeing problems that may not exist: A reply to West et al.’s (2018) questioning of the procedural deficit hypothesis. Development Science, 22(4), e12814.
Conway, C. M., & Pisoni, D. B. (2008). Neurocognitive basis of implicit learning of sequential structure and its relation to language processing. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1145(1), 113-131. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1416.009.
Del Re, A., & Hoyt, W. T. (2018). MAc: Meta-analysis with correlations. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=MAc.
Delage, H., & Frauenfelder, U. H. (2020). Relationship between working memory and complex syntax in children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Child Language, 47(3), 600-632. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000722.
*Deroost, N., Zeischka, P., Coomans, D., Bouazza, S., Depessemier, P., & Soetens, E. (2010). Intact first- and second-order implicit sequence learning in secondary-school-aged children with developmental dyslexia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(6), 561-572. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390903313556.
*Desmottes, L., Meulemans, T., & Maillart, C. (2016). Later learning stages in procedural memory are impaired in children with specific language impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 48, 53-68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2015.10.010.
*Desmottes, L., Maillart, C., & Meulemans, T. (2017a). Memory consolidation in children with specific language impairment: Delayed gains and susceptibility to interference in implicit sequence learning. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 39(3), 265-285. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2016.1223279.
*Desmottes, L., Meulemans, T., Patinec, M. A., & Maillart, C. (2017b). Distributed training enhances implicit sequence acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(9), 2636-2647. https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-16-0146.
Earle, F. S., & Ullman, M. T. (2021). Deficits of Learning in Procedural Memory and Consolidation in Declarative Memory in Adults With Developmental Language Disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 64(February), 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_jslhr-20-00292.
Erickson, L. C., Kaschak, M. P., Thiessen, E. D., & Berry, C. A. S. (2016). Individual differences in statistical learning: Conceptual and measurement issues. Collabra, 2(1), 14. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.41.
Fawcett, A. J., Nicolson, R. I., & Dean, P. (1996). Impaired performance of children with dyslexia on a range of cerebellar tasks. Annals of Dyslexia, 46(1), 259-283. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02648179.
Fisher, Z., & Tipton, E. (2015). robumeta: An R-package for robust variance estimation in meta-analysis. ArXiv:1503.02220 [Stat]. Retrieved from https://arxiv.org/abs/1503.02220.
Fleiss, J. L. (1986). The design and analysis of clinical experiments. John Wiley & Sons.
Fostick, L., & Revah, H. (2018). Dyslexia as a multi-deficit disorder: Working memory and auditory temporal processing. Acta Psychologica, 183, 19-28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.12.010.
*Gabay, Y., Schiff, R., & Vakil, E. (2012). Dissociation between the procedural learning of letter names and motor sequences in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 50(10), 2435-2441. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.06.014.
*Gabriel, A., Maillart, C., Guillaume, M., Stefaniak, N., & Meulemans, T. (2011). Exploration of serial structure procedural learning in children with language impairment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(2), 336-343. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617710001724.
*Gabriel, A., Maillart, C., Stefaniak, N., Lejeune, C., Desmottes, L., & Meulemans, T. (2013). Procedural learning in specific language impairment: Effects of sequence complexity. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19(3), 264-271. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617712001270.
*Gabriel, A., Meulemans, T., Parisse, C., & Maillart, C. (2015). Procedural learning across modalities in French-speaking children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(3), 747-769. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716413000490.
*Gabriel, A., Stefaniak, N., Maillart, C., Schmitz, X., & Meulemans, T. (2012). Procedural visual learning in children with specific language impairment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 21(4), 329-341. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11-0044).
Galbraith, R. F. (1988). A note on graphical presentation of estimated odds ratios from several clinical trials. Statistics in medicine, 7(8), 889-894.
Hamrick, P., Lum, J. A. G., & Ullman, M. T. (2018). Child first language and adult second language are both tied to general-purpose learning systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(7), 1487-1492. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1713975115.
*Hedenius, M., Lum, J. A. G., & Bölte, S. (2021). Alterations of procedural memory consolidation in children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychology, 35(2), 185-196. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000708.
*Hedenius, M., Persson, J., Alm, P. A., Ullman, M. T., Howard, J. H., Howard, D. V., & Jennische, M. (2013). Impaired implicit sequence learning in children with developmental dyslexia. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(11), 3924-3935. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.08.014.
Hedge, C., Powell, G., & Sumner, P. (2018). The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behavior Research Methods, 50(3), 1166-1186. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0935-1.
Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Academic Press.
Hedges, L. V., Tipton, E., & Johnson, M. C. (2010). Robust variance estimation in meta-regression with dependent effect size estimates. Research Synthesis Methods, 1(1), 39-65. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.5.
*Henderson, L. M., & Warmington, M. (2017). A sequence learning impairment in dyslexia? It depends on the task. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 60, 198-210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2016.11.002.
Higgins, J. P. T., & Thompson, S. G. (2002). Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 21(11), 1539-1558. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.1186.
Hill, E. L. (2001). Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: A review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36(2), 149-171. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820010019874.
*Hsu, H. J., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2014). Sequence-specific procedural learning deficits in children with specific language impairment. Developmental Science, 17(3), 352-365. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12125.
Jackson, E., Leitão, S., Claessen, M., & Boyes, M. (2020). Working, declarative, and procedural memory in children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63(12), 4162-4178. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-00135.
Janacsek, K., Shattuck, K. F., Tagarelli, K. M., Lum, J. A. G., Turkeltaub, P. E., & Ullman, M. T. (2020). Sequence learning in the human brain: A functional neuroanatomical meta-analysis of serial reaction time studies. NeuroImage, 207, Article 116387. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116387.
Kalra, P. B., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Finn, A. S. (2019). Evidence of stable individual differences in implicit learning. Cognition, 190, 199-211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.05.007.
Keele, S. W., Ivry, R., Mayr, U., Hazeltine, E., & Heuer, H. (2003). The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. Psychological Review, 110(2), 316-339. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.2.316.
*Kidd, E. (2012). Implicit statistical learning is directly associated with the acquisition of syntax. Developmental Psychology, 48(1), 171-184. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025405.
*Kidd, E., & Kirjavainen, M. (2011). Investigating the contribution of procedural and declarative memory to the acquisition of past tense morphology: Evidence from Finnish. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(4-6), 794-829. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2010.493735.
Kirkham, J. J., Dwan, K. M., Altman, D. G., Gamble, C., Dodd, S., Smyth, R., & Williamson, P. R. (2010). The impact of outcome reporting bias in randomised controlled trials on a cohort of systematic reviews. BMJ, 340, c365. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.c365.
Krishnan, S., Watkins, K. E., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2016). Neurobiological basis of language learning difficulties. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(9), 701-714. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.06.012.
Kottner, J., & Streiner, D. L. (2011). The difference between reliability and agreement. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 64(6), 701-702. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.12.001.
*Kuppuraj, S., Rao, P., & Bishop, D. V. (2016). Declarative capacity does not trade-off with procedural capacity in children with specific language impairment. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments, 1, 1-17. https://doi.org/10.1177/2396941516674416.
*Lammertink, I., Boersma, P., Wijnen, F., & Rispens, J. (2017). Statistical learning in specific language impairment: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(12), 3474-3486. https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-16-0439.
Lammertink, I., Boersma, P., Wijnen, F., & Rispens, J. (2020). Statistical learning in the visuomotor domain and its relation to grammatical proficiency in children with and without developmental language disorder: A conceptual replication and meta-analysis. Language Learning and Development, 16(4), 426-450. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2020.1820340.
*Lee, J. C., & Tomblin, J. B. (2015). Procedural learning and individual differences in language. Language Learning and Development, 11(3), 215-236. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2014.904168.
Light, R. J., Richard, J., Light, R., & Pillemer, D. B. (1984). Summing up: The science of reviewing research. Harvard University Press.
*Llompart, M., & Dąbrowska, E. (2020). Explicit but not implicit memory predicts ultimate attainment in the native language. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1-14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.569586.
Loken, E., & Gelman, A. (2017). Measurement error and the replication crisis. Science, 355(6325), 584-585. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aal3618.
*Lukács, Á., & Kemény, F. (2014). Domain-general sequence learning deficit in specific language impairment. Neuropsychology, 28(3), 472-483. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000052.
Lum, J. A. G., Conti-Ramsden, G., Morgan, A. T., & Ullman, M. T. (2014). Procedural learning deficits in specific language impairment (SLI): A meta-analysis of serial reaction time task performance. Cortex, 51(100), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.10.011.
*Lum, J. A. G., Conti-Ramsden, G., Page, D., & Ullman, M. T. (2012). Working, declarative and procedural memory in specific language impairment. Cortex, 48(9), 1138-1154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.06.001.
*Lum, J. A. G., & Kidd, E. (2012). An examination of the associations among multiple memory systems, past tense, and vocabulary in typically developing 5-year-old children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55(4), 989-1006. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0137).
Lum, J. A. G., Ullman, M. T., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2013). Procedural learning is impaired in dyslexia: Evidence from a meta-analysis of serial reaction time studies. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(10), 3460-3476. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.07.017.
Marini, A., Piccolo, B., Taverna, L., Berginc, M., & Ozbič, M. (2020). The complex relation between executive functions and language in preschoolers with developmental language disorders. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(5), 1772. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17051772.
Mathews, R. C. (1997). Is research painting a biased picture of implicit learning? The dangers of methodological purity in scientific debate. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4(1), 38-42. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210771.
*Mayor-Dubois, C., Zesiger, P., Van der Linden, M., & Roulet-Perez, E. (2014). Nondeclarative learning in children with specific language impairment: Predicting regularities in the visuomotor, phonological, and cognitive domains. Child Neuropsychology, 20(1), 14-22. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2012.734293.
*Menghini, D., Finzi, A., Benassi, M., Bolzani, R., Facoetti, A., Giovagnoli, S., Ruffino, M., & Vicari, S. (2010). Different underlying neurocognitive deficits in developmental dyslexia: A comparative study. Neuropsychologia, 48(4), 863-872.
*Mimeau, C., Coleman, M., & Donlan, C. (2016). The role of procedural memory in grammar and numeracy skills. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 28(8), 899-908.
Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (2011). Dyslexia, dysgraphia, procedural learning and the cerebellum. Cortex, 47(1), 117-127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.08.016.
Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19(1), 1-32. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8.
Novick, M. R. (1966). The axioms and principal results of classical test theory. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 3(1), 1-18. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-2496(66)90002-2.
*Oliveira, C. M., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., & Henderson, L. M. (2022, May 10). Reliability of the Serial Reaction Time task: If at first you don't succeed, try try try again. PsyArxiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/hqmy7.
Oliveira, C. M., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., & Henderson, L. M. (2023, April 25). The reliability of the serial reaction time task: Meta-analysis of test-retest correlation. PsyArxiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/bjwqk.
Packard, M. G., & Knowlton, B. J. (2002). Learning and memory functions of the basal ganglia. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 25(1), 563-593. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142937.
Park, J., Miller, C. A., Rosenbaum, D. A., Sanjeevan, T., van Hell, J. G., Weiss, D. J., & Mainela-Arnold, E. (2018). Bilingualism and procedural learning in typically developing children and children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61(3), 634-644. https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-16-0409.
Parsons, S., Kruijt, A.-W., & Fox, E. (2019). Psychological science needs a standard practice of reporting the reliability of cognitive-behavioral measurements. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 2(4), 378-395. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245919879695.
Pascual-Leone, A., Wassermann, E. M., Grafman, J., & Hallett, M. (1996). The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in implicit procedural learning. Experimental Brain Research, 107(3), 479-485. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00230427.
Pustejovsky, J. (2021). clubSandwich: Cluster-robust (sandwich) variance estimators with small-sample corrections. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=clubSandwich.
Pustejovsky, J. E., & Tipton, E. (2022). Meta-analysis with Robust Variance Estimation: Expanding the Range of Working Models. Prevention Science, 23(3), 425-438. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-021-01246-3.
Robertson, E. M., Tormos, J. M., & Maeda, F. (2001). The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during sequence learning is specific for spatial information. Cerebral Cortex, 11(7), 628-635.
Romani, C., Tsouknida, E., di Betta, A. M., & Olson, A. (2011). Reduced attentional capacity, but normal processing speed and shifting of attention in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from a serial task. Cortex, 47(6), 715-733. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2010.05.008.
Rouder, J. N., Kumar, A., & Haaf, J. M. (2019, March 25). Why most studies of individual differences with inhibition tasks are bound to fail. PsyArxiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/3cjr5.
Saffran, J. R. (2020). Statistical language learning in infancy. Child Development Perspectives, 14(1), 49-54.
*Schmalz, X., Moll, K., Mulatti, C., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2019). Is statistical learning ability related to reading ability, and if so, why? Scientific Studies of Reading, 23(1), 64-76. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2018.1482304.
Schwarb, H., & Schumacher, E. (2012). Generalized lessons about sequence learning from the study of the serial reaction time task. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 8(2), 165-178. https://doi.org/10.5709/acp-0113-1.
Sengottuvel, K., & Rao, P. K. S. (2013). An adapted serial reaction time task for sequence learning measurements. Psychological Studies, 58(3), 276-284. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12646-013-0204-z.
Shanks, D. R., & St John, M. F. (1994). Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17(3), 367-395. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00035032.
Siegelman, N., Bogaerts, L., Christiansen, M. H., & Frost, R. (2017). Towards a theory of individual differences in statistical learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences B, 372, 20160059. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0059.
* Siegelman, N., & Frost, R. (2015). Statistical learning as an individual ability: Theoretical perspectives and empirical evidence. Journal of Memory and Language, 81, 105-120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2015.02.001.
Smith, L. B., Suanda, S. H., & Yu, C. (2014). The unrealized promise of infant statistical word-referent learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(5), 251-258.
Snowling, M. (2000). Dyslexia (2nd ed). Blackwell.
Snowling, M. J., Hulme, C., & Nation, K. (2020). Defining and understanding dyslexia: Past, present and future. Oxford Review of Education, 46(4), 501-513. https://doi.org/10.1080/03054985.2020.1765756.
Spearman, C. (1904). “General intelligence,” objectively determined and measured. American Journal of Psychology, 15(2), 201-292.
Stanley, T. D., & Doucouliagos, H. (2014). Meta-regression approximations to reduce publication selection bias. Research Synthesis Methods, 5(1), 60-78. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1095.
Stark-Inbar, A., Raza, M., Taylor, J. A., & Ivry, R. B. (2017). Individual differences in implicit motor learning: Task specificity in sensorimotor adaptation and sequence learning. Journal of Neurophysiology, 117(1), 412-428. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01141.2015.
*Spit, S., & Rispens, J. (2019). On the relation between procedural learning and syntactic proficiency in gifted children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 48(2), 417-429. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-018-9611-6.
*Stoodley, C. J., Harrison, E. P. D., & Stein, J. F. (2006). Implicit motor learning deficits in dyslexic adults. Neuropsychologia, 44(5), 795-798. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.07.009.
Tanner-Smith, E. E., Tipton, E., & Polanin, J. R. (2016). Handling complex meta-analytic data structures using robust variance estimates: A tutorial in R. Journal of Developmental and Life-Course Criminology, 2(1), 85-112. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40865-016-0026-5.
Tipton, E., & Pustejovsky, J. E. (2015). Small-sample adjustments for tests of moderators and model fit using robust variance estimation in meta-regression. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 40(6), 604-634. https://doi.org/10.3102/1076998615606099.
Torriero, S., Oliveri, M., Koch, G., Caltagirone, C., & Petrosini, L. (2004). Interference of left and right cerebellar rTMS with procedural learning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(9), 1605-1611. https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929042568488.
Turner, R. M., Bird, S. M., & Higgins, J. P. T. (2013). The impact of study size on meta-analyses: Examination of underpowered studies in Cochrane reviews. PLoS ONE, 8(3), e59202. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059202.
Ullman, M. T. (2001a). The declarative/procedural model of lexicon and grammar. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(1), 37-69. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005204207369.
Ullman, M. T. (2001b). A neurocognitive perspective on language: The declarative/procedural model. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(10), 717-726. https://doi.org/10.1038/35094573.
Ullman, M. T. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92(1-2), 231-270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.008.
Ullman, M. T. (2015). The declarative/procedural model: A neurobiologically motivated theory of first and second language. In B. VanPatten & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction (2nd edition, pp. 135-158). Routledge.
Ullman, M. T. (2016a). The declarative/procedural model. In G. Hickok & S. L. Small (Eds.), Neurobiology of language (pp. 953-968). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407794-2.00076-6.
Ullman, M. T. (2016b). Chapter 76-The declarative/procedural model: A neurobiological model of language learning, knowledge, and use. In G. Hickok & S. L. Small (Eds.), Neurobiology of language (pp. 953-968). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407794-2.00076-6.
Ullman, M. T., Earle, F. S., Walenski, M., & Janacsek, K. (2020). The neurocognition of developmental disorders of language. Annual Review of Psychology, 71(1), 389-417. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011555.
Ullman, M. T., & Pierpont, E. I. (2005). Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41(3), 399-433. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70276-4.
Ullman, M. T., & Pullman, M. Y. (2015). A compensatory role for declarative memory in neurodevelopmental disorders. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 51, 205-222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.008.
* Vakil, E., Lowe, M., & Goldfus, C. (2015). Performance of children with developmental dyslexia on two skill learning tasks-Serial Reaction Time and Tower of Hanoi Puzzle: A test of the specific procedural learning difficulties theory. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 48(5), 471-481. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413508981.
van der Lely, H. K. J. (2005). Domain-specific cognitive systems: Insight from Grammatical-SLI. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 53-59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.002.
van der Lely, H. K. J., & Pinker, S. (2014). The biological basis of language: Insight from developmental grammatical impairments. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(11), 586-595. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.07.001.
* van Witteloostuijn, M., Boersma, P., Wijnen, F., & Rispens, J. (2019). Statistical learning abilities of children with dyslexia across three experimental paradigms. PLoS ONE, 14(8), Article e0220041. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0220041.
Vicari, S., Marotta, L., Menghini, D., Molinari, M., & Petrosini, L. (2003). Implicit learning deficit in children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 41(1), 108-114. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00082-9.
Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor. Journal of Statistical Software, 36(3), 1-48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v036.i03.
von Hippel, P. T. (2015). The heterogeneity statistic I2 can be biased in small meta-analyses. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 15, (1), 35. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12874-015-0024-z.
*West, G., Clayton, F. J., Shanks, D. R., & Hulme, C. (2019). Procedural and declarative learning in dyslexia. Dyslexia, 25(3), 246-255. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1615.
West, G., Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2021). Is a procedural learning deficit a causal risk factor for developmental language disorder or dyslexia? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 57(5), 749-770. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001172.
*West, G., Shanks, D. R., & Hulme, C. (2021). Sustained attention, not procedural learning, is a predictor of reading, language and arithmetic skills in children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 25(1), 47-63. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2020.1750618.
*West, G., Vadillo, M. A., Shanks, D. R., & Hulme, C. (2018). The procedural learning deficit hypothesis of language learning disorders: We see some problems. Developmental Science, 21(2), e12552. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12552.
Wiernik, B. M., & Dahlke, J. A. (2019). Obtaining unbiased results in meta-analysis: The importance of correcting for statistical artefacts [Preprint]. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/9mpbn.
Williams, J. N. (2020). The neuroscience of implicit learning. Language Learning, 70(S2), 255-307. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12405. - Contributed Indexing: Keywords: Developmental language disorder; Dyslexia; Individual differences; Language; Literacy; Procedural memory; Sequence learning; Specific language impairment
- الموضوع: Date Created: 20230704 Date Completed: 20230706 Latest Revision: 20230718
- الموضوع: 20231215
- الرقم المعرف: 10.1111/cogs.13310
- الرقم المعرف: 37401920
- المصدر:
حقوق النشر© 2024، دائرة الثقافة والسياحة جميع الحقوق محفوظة Powered By EBSCO Stacks 3.3.0 [353] | Staff Login
حقوق النشر © دائرة الثقافة والسياحة، جميع الحقوق محفوظة
No Comments.