Item request has been placed! ×
Item request cannot be made. ×
loading  Processing Request

Theory of mind development from adolescence to adulthood: Testing the two-component model.

Item request has been placed! ×
Item request cannot be made. ×
loading   Processing Request
  • معلومة اضافية
    • المصدر:
      Publisher: Wiley-Blackwell Country of Publication: England NLM ID: 8308022 Publication Model: Print-Electronic Cited Medium: Internet ISSN: 2044-835X (Electronic) Linking ISSN: 0261510X NLM ISO Abbreviation: Br J Dev Psychol Subsets: MEDLINE
    • بيانات النشر:
      Publication: <2012-> : Chichester : Wiley-Blackwell
      Original Publication: Leicester [Leicestershire] : British Psychological Society, c1983-
    • الموضوع:
    • نبذة مختصرة :
      The ability to infer mental and affective states of others is crucial for social functioning. This ability, denoted as Theory of Mind (ToM), develops rapidly during childhood, yet results on its development across adolescence and into young adulthood are rare. In the present study, we tested the two-component model, measuring age-related changes in social-perceptual and social-cognitive ToM in a sample of 267 participants between 11 and 25 years of age. Additionally, we measured language, reasoning, and inhibitory control as major covariates. Participants inferred mental states from non-verbal cues in a social-perceptual task (Eye Test) and from stories with faux pas in a social-cognitive task (Faux Pas Test). Results showed substantial improvement across adolescence in both ToM measures and in the covariates. Analysis with linear mixed models (LMM) revealed specific age-related growth for the social-perceptual component, while the age-related increase of the social-cognitive component fully aligned with the increase of the covariates. These results support the distinction between ToM components and indicate that adolescence is a crucial period for developing social-perceptual ToM abilities. Statement of contribution What is already known on this subject? To date, much research has been dedicated to Theory of Mind (ToM) development in early and middle childhood. However, only a few studies have examined development of ToM in adolescence. Studies so far suggest age-related differences in ToM between adolescents and young adults. What this study adds The study offers several methodological advantages including a large sample size with a continuous distribution of age (age 11-25) and the use of a comprehensive test battery to assess ToM and covariates (language, executive functions, reasoning). The results provide evidence for asymmetries in the development of two ToM components (social-perceptual and social-cognitive; the two-component account) across the studied age range: the social perceptual component showed specific development, while the age-related increase of the social-cognitive component fully aligned with increase of the covariates. Adolescence is a crucial period for developing social-perceptual ToM abilities.
      (© 2019 The Authors. British Journal of Developmental Psychology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of British Psychological Society.)
    • References:
      Aïte, A., Cassotti, M., Linzarini, A., Osmont, A., Houdé, O., & Borst, G. (2018). Adolescents’inhibitory control: Keep it cool or lose control. Developmental Science, 21(1), e12491. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12491.
      Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1311-1320. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.5.1311.
      Baker, C. A., Peterson, E., Pulos, S., & Kirkland, R. A. (2014). Eyes and IQ: A meta-analysis of the relationship between intelligence and “Reading the Mind in the Eyes”. Intelligence, 44, 78-92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2014.03.001.
      Banerjee, R., Watling, D., & Caputi, M. (2011). Peer relations and the understanding of faux pas: Longitudinal evidence for bidirectional associations. Child Development, 82, 1887-1905. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01669.x.
      Baron-Cohen, S., O’riordan, M., Stone, V., Jones, R., & Plaisted, K. (1999). Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 407-418. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023035012436.
      Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The "Reading the Mind in the Eyes" test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 241-251. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00715.
      Barton, K. (2013). MuMIn: Multi-model inference. R package version 1.9.13. Vienna, Austria: Comprehensive R Archive Network (CRAN).
      Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1-48. arXiv:1406.5823 [stat.CO].
      Bölte, S. (2005). Reading Mind in the Eyes Test für Erwachsene (dt. Fassung) von S. Baron-Cohen [Reading the mind in the eyes test for adults (German version) by S. Baron-Cohen]. Frankfurt, Germany: J. W. Goethe Universität Frankfurt/M: Eigendruck.
      Bosco, F. M., Gabbatore, I., & Tirassa, M. (2014). A broad assessment of theory of mind in adolescence: The complexity of mindreading. Consciousness and Cognition, 24, 84-97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.01.003.
      Brizio, A., Gabbatore, I., Tirassa, M., & Bosco, F. M. (2015). “No more a child, not yet an adult”: Studying social cognition in adolescence. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1011. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01011.
      Carpendale, J. I., & Lewis, C. (2004). Constructing an understanding of mind: The development of children's social understanding within social interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 79-96. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X04000032.
      Collins, W. A., & Laursen, B. (2004). Changing relationships, changing youth: Interpersonal contexts of adolescent development. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 24, 55-62. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431603260882.
      Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2013). Silent films and strange stories: Theory of mind, gender, and social experiences in middle childhood. Child Development, 84, 989-1003. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12017.
      Dumontheil, I., Apperly, I. A., & Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. Developmental Science, 13, 331-338. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00888.x.
      Fuhrmann, D., Knoll, L. J., & Blakemore, S. J. (2015). Adolescence as a sensitive period of brain development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 558-566. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.008.
      Fuhrmann, D., Knoll, L. J., Sakhardande, A. L., Speekenbrink, M., Kadosh, K. C., & Blakemore, S. J. (2016). Perception and recognition of faces in adolescence. Scientific Reports, 6, 33497. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep33497.
      Garcia, N. V., & Scherf, K. S. (2015). Emerging sensitivity to socially complex expressions: A unique role for adolescence? Child Development Perspectives, 9, 84-90. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12114.
      Geiger, A., Bente, G., Lammers, S., Tepest, R., Roth, D., Bzdok, D., & Vogeley, K. (2019). Distinct functional roles of the mirror neuron system and the mentalizing system. NeuroImage, 202, 116102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116102.
      Harris, P. L., de Rosnay, M., & Pons, F. (2005). Language and children's understanding of mental states. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 69-73. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00337.x.
      Hill, E. L. (2004). Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental Review, 24, 189-233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2004.01.001.
      Hughes, C., & Devine, R. T. (2015). Individual differences in theory of mind from preschool to adolescence: Achievements and directions. Child Development Perspectives, 9, 149-153. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12124.
      Humphrey, G., & Dumontheil, I. (2016). Development of risk-taking, perspective-taking, and inhibitory control during adolescence. Developmental Neuropsychology, 41, 59-76. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2016.1161764.
      Im-Bolter, N., Agostino, A., & Owens-Jaffray, K. (2016). Theory of mind in middle childhood and early adolescence: Different from before? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 149, 98-115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.12.006.
      Kennedy, D. P., & Adolphs, R. (2012). The social brain in psychiatric and neurological disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 559-572. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.09.006.
      Larson, R. W., & Verma, S. (1999). How children and adolescents spend time across the world: Work, play, and developmental opportunities. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 701-736. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.701.
      Lawson, J., Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). Empathising and systemising in adults with and without Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 301-310. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JADD.0000029552.42724.1b.
      Lehrl, S. (2005). MWT-B Mehrfachwahl Wortschatz-Intelligenztest. Balingen, Germany: Spitta-Verlag.
      Lilliefors, H. W. (1967). On the Kolmogorov-Smirnov Test for normality with mean and variance unknown. Journal of American Statistical Association, 62, 399-402. https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1967.10482916.
      Luengo Kanacri, B. P., Pastorelli, C., Eisenberg, N., Zuffianò, A., & Caprara, G. V. (2013). The development of prosociality from adolescence to early adulthood: the role of effortful control. Journal of Personality, 81, 302-312. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12001.
      McCulloch, C. E., Searle, S. R. (2001). Generalized, linear, and mixed models. Series in probability and statistics. New York, NY: Wiley.
      Meinhardt-Injac, B., Daum, M. M., Meinhardt, G., & Persike, M. (2018). The two-systems account of Theory of Mind: Testing the links to social-perceptual and cognitive abilities. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 25. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00025.
      Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78, 622-646. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01018.x.
      Molenaar, P. C., Huizenga, H. M., & Nesselroade, J. R. (2003). The relationship between the structure of interindividual and intraindividual variability: A theoretical and empirical vindication of developmental systems theory. In U. M. Staudinger & U. Lindenberger (Eds.), Understanding human development. Boston, MA: Springer.
      Motta-Mena, N. V., & Scherf, K. S. (2016). Pubertal development shapes perception of complex facial expressions. Developmental Science, 20, e12451. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12451.
      Osterhaus, C., Koerber, S., & Sodian, B. (2016). Scaling of advanced theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 87, 1971-1991. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12566.
      Peterson, C. C., & Wellman, H. M. (2018). Longitudinal theory of mind (ToM) development from preschool to adolescence with and without ToM delay. Child Development, 90, 1917-1934. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13064.
      Pinheiro, J. C., & Bates, D. M. (2000). Linear mixed-effects models: basic concepts and examples. In Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS. Statistics and computing. New York, NY: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/0-387-22747-4_1.
      Pyers, J. E., & Senghas, A. (2009). Language promotes false-belief understanding: Evidence from learners of a new sign language. Psychological Science, 20, 805-812. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02377.x.
      R Core Team. (2012). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
      Radecki, M. A., Cox, S. R., & MacPherson, S. E. (2019). Theory of Mind and psychosocial characteristics in older men. Psychology and Aging, 34, 145-151. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000324.
      Rakoczy, H. (2017). Theory of Mind. In B. Hopkins, E. Geangu, & S. Linkenauger (Eds.), The Cambridge Encyclopedia of child development (pp. 505-512). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316216491.081.
      Raven, J. (2000). The Raven's progressive matrices: Change and stability over culture and time. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 1-48. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0735.
      Rice, K., Anderson, L. C., Velnoskey, K., Thompson, J. C., & Redcay, E. (2016). Biological motion perception links diverse facets of theory of mind during middle childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 146, 238-246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.003.
      Rodger, H., Vizioli, L., Ouyang, X., & Caldara, R. (2015). Mapping the development of facial expression recognition. Developmental Science, 18, 926-939. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12281.
      Schlaffke, L., Lissek, S., Lenz, M., Juckel, G., Schultz, T., Tegenthoff, M., … Brüne, M. (2015). Shared and nonshared neural networks of cognitive and affective theory-of-mind: A neuroimaging study using cartoon picture stories. Human Brain Mapping, 36(1), 29-39. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22610.
      Schmitz, T. W., Cheng, F. H., & De Rosa, E. (2010). Failing to ignore: PARADOXICAL neural effects of perceptual load on early attentional selection in normal aging. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 14750-14758. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2687-10.2010.
      Schneider, D., Slaughter, V. P., & Dux, P. E. (2015). What do we know about implicit false-belief tracking? Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 22, 1-12. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0644-z.
      Schuwerk, T., Vuori, M., & Sodian, B. (2015). Implicit and explicit theory of mind reasoning in autism spectrum disorders: the impact of experience. Autism, 19, 459-468. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361314526004.
      Sebastian, C. L., Fontaine, N. M., Bird, G., Blakemore, S. J., De Brito, S. A., McCrory, E. J., & Viding, E. (2011). Neural processing associated with cognitive and affective Theory of Mind in adolescents and adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 53-63. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr023.
      Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2007). Dissociable prefrontal networks for cognitive and affective theory of mind: A lesion study. Neuropsychologia, 45, 3054-3067. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.021.
      Slaughter, V., Imuta, K., Peterson, C. C., & Henry, J. D. (2015). Meta-analysis of theory of mind and peer popularity in the preschool and early school years. Child Development, 86, 1159-1174. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12372.
      Steinberg, L. (2005). Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 69-74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.005.
      Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 640-656. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892998562942.
      Symeonidou, I., Dumontheil, I., Chow, W. Y., & Breheny, R. (2016). Development of online use of theory of mind during adolescence: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 149, 81-97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.007.
      Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (2000). A componential view of theory of mind: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognition, 76, 59-90. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00069-X.
      Tamnes, C. K., Overbye, K., Ferschmann, L., Fjell, A. M., Walhovd, K. B., Blakemore, S. J., & Dumontheil, I. (2018). Social perspective taking is associated with self-reported prosocial behavior and regional cortical thickness across adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 54, 1745-1757.
      Thomas, L. A., De Bellis, M. D., Graham, R., & LaBar, K. S. (2007). Development of emotional facial recognition in late childhood and adolescence. Developmental Science, 10, 547-558. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00614.x.
      Tousignant, B., Sirois, K., Achim, A. M., Massicotte, E., & Jackson, P. L. (2017). A comprehensive assessment of social cognition from adolescence to adulthood. Cognitive Development, 43, 214-223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.05.001.
      Valle, A., Massaro, D., Castelli, I., & Marchetti, A. (2015). Theory of mind development in adolescence and early adulthood: The growing complexity of recursive thinking ability. Europe's Journal of Psychology, 11, 112-124. https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v11i1.829.
      Vetter, N. C., Leipold, K., Kliegel, M., Phillips, L. H., & Altgassen, M. (2013). Ongoing development of social cognition in adolescence. Child Neuropsychology, 19, 615-629. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2012.718324.
      Wellman, H. M., Fang, F., & Peterson, C. C. (2011). Sequential progressions in a theory-of-mind scale: Longitudinal perspectives. Child Development, 82, 780-792. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01583.x.
      White, S., Hill, E., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2009). Revisiting the strange stories: Revealing mentalizing impairments in autism. Child Development, 80, 1097-1117. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01319.x.
      Zerwas, S., Balaraman, G., & Brownell, C. (2004). Constructing an understanding of mind with peers. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 130-130. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X04590039.
    • Contributed Indexing:
      Keywords: Theory of Mind; adolescence; socio-emotional development; two-component model; young adulthood
    • الموضوع:
      Date Created: 20200122 Date Completed: 20210611 Latest Revision: 20210611
    • الموضوع:
      20231215
    • الرقم المعرف:
      10.1111/bjdp.12320
    • الرقم المعرف:
      31960462